Dividing Attention and Metacognition

Dividing Attention and Metacognition

Yaoping Peng, Jonathan G. Tullis
Copyright: © 2022 |Pages: 29
DOI: 10.4018/978-1-7998-9243-4.ch004
OnDemand:
(Individual Chapters)
Available
$37.50
No Current Special Offers
TOTAL SAVINGS: $37.50

Abstract

Students increasingly control their learning as university instructors shift away from lecture formats, courses are offered online, and the internet offers near infinite resources for student-controlled informal learning. Students typically make effective choices about learning, including what to learn, when to learn, and how to learn, but sometimes make less-than-optimal study choices, including trying to study while multi-tasking. Dividing attention among various tasks impairs both learning and learners' control over their learning because secondary tasks divert cognitive resources away from learning and metacognition. This chapter reviews recent studies explaining how dividing attention affects students' metacognition, including their assessments of their own learning and the study choices that they make. This chapter reviews the fundamentals of metacognition, describes the impact of dividing attention on the effectiveness of learners' metacognition, and provides suggestions about how to enhance the efficacy of metacognition when students' attentional resources are limited.
Chapter Preview
Top

Introduction

Digital technologies are prevalent in college classrooms and, consequently, they easily and increasingly distract learners from classroom activities. Digital devices, like laptops, smartphones, and tablets, can support learning; for example, they can help students take and store notes and keep deadlines organized in calendars. However, digital technologies can also allow students to perform various course-unrelated activities, including texting friends, posting and viewing social media, checking email, browsing videos on TikTok, reading news and current events, shopping online, and playing video games. A vast majority of students describe being distracted by their digital technologies during face-to-face classes: over 95% of students report sending text massages at least once per class, and over 19% of undergraduate students report constantly texting while in class (le Roux & Parry, 2018). Similarly, when internet use was tracked during a lecture, undergraduate students spent an average of 37% of class time on non-academic tasks (Ravizza, Uitvlugt, & Fenn, 2017). In online classes, which have experienced huge enrollment growths recently (Allen, Seaman, Poulin, & Straut, 2016), distractions happen even more frequently than in traditional face-to-face classes (Lepp, Barkley, Karpinski, & Singh, 2019). Several reasons contribute to the high rate of digital distractions in class, including learners’ needs to build and maintain social connections (David et al., 2015), reduce anxiety and boredom (Wang & Tchernev, 2012), and seek out information about tasks unrelated to the central learning task (Bellur et al., 2013).

Digital distractions are increasing in college classrooms, as younger generations report attempting to divide their attention between the primary learning tasks and digital technologies more frequently than older generations (Carrier, Cheever, Rosen, Benitez, & Chang, 2009; Carrier, Rosen, Cheever, & Lim, 2015). Digital distractions may be increasing for several reasons. First, younger generations have been saturated with digital technologies from young ages (i.e., they are digital “natives”), such that they can navigate through digital technologies easily (Thompson, 2013). Second, the development and proliferation of attention-grabbing and attention-demanding digital technologies may enable and prompt students to be distracted at an increasing rate. The continued advances in digital hardware afford students the ability to combine multiple digital activities easily; stronger CPUs allow several computer programs to run simultaneously and provide quicker response speeds to switch smoothly between tasks, and increased screen resolution helps show multiple viewable windows at once (Gibson, 1979; Wijekumar, Meyer, Wgoner & Ferguson, 2006). Finally, the algorithms used by social media companies to prioritize posts are constantly evolving so that they more effectively attract users’ attention and make their platforms even more difficult to ignore (Agrawal, 2016; Stern, 2021).

Key Terms in this Chapter

Metacognitive Control: Learners’ choices about how to study, including what information they pay attention to, whether they engage in distractions, which study strategies they use, how much effort they expend to learn material, and when they study.

Fluency: Learners’ feelings of ease of processing during learning, including how easily they learn materials and how easily information comes to mind.

Divided Attention: Splitting attentional resources between two different tasks.

Metacognition: Learners’ assessments of their learning and the study choices that learners make based upon their assessments.

Metacognitive Monitoring: Assessments of one’s own learning, including learners’ judgments about how well they understand instruction, the confidence of their answers on a test, and predictions about how likely they will be able to recall information later.

Scaffolding: Support or guidance provided to learners that can decrease the necessary mental resources needed to accomplish a task.

Attention: Cognitive process that chooses and selectively concentrates on some stimuli and ignores others.

Digital Distractions: Breaks in concentration and attention away from the primary task due to electronic technologies and media.

Metacognitive Cues: Evidence (e.g., fluency, past test performance, learning materials, study time, learning strategies) that learners use to infer how well they have learned.

Working Memory Capacity: The amount of information learners are able to keep in mind and work with at a time.

Complete Chapter List

Search this Book:
Reset